Frequently Asked Questions

Below you can find a list of frequently asked questions, organized by topic, that reach us via email. Click the question to see our response.

Causality

The short answer is: It depends on whether the assumptions underlying RI-CLPMs hold. In practice, the cross-lagged effects that are obtained with the RI-CLPM (as well as with other cross-lagged models) are often interpreted as causal effects, sometimes quite explicitly, but oftentimes in a more implicit way through the use of specific language (e.g., when one variable is described to “react to”, “respond to”, “impact”, or “spill over into” another variable). Such an interpretation is valid if all assumptions underlying the analysis hold, which includes both parametric assumptions such as linearity and no interactions; and causal identification assumptions such as sequential exchangeability, consistency, and positivity. However, in applications of the RI-CLPM, these assumptions are rarely subject to explicit scrutiny, and therefore it is unclear if a causal interpretation of effects is valid (e.g., it is stated that under these assumptions the results can be interpreted causally, without a thorough discussion of the plausibility of these assumptions).

Furthermore, there is more general criticism of the use of SEM models for causal inference by researchers from disciplines like epidemiology and biostatistics. They point out that SEM models depend heavily on parametric assumptions; since these are likely to be violated—at least to some degree—in practice, SEM is prone to bias when used for causal inference. The use of methods that have been developed in the potential outcomes framework is therefore recommended. Compared to SEM, these alternative methods rely less on parametric assumptions therefore should, in principle, lead to more robust causal inference. For a didactical introduction to this discussion, and more information on the role of cross-lagged panel research in causal inference, we refer the reader to Mulder, Luijken, Penning de Vries, and Hamaker (2024). We conclude that increased reliance on parametric assumptions (as in cross-lagged panel modeling) does not always translate to increased bias (even under mild model misspecification), researchers are still well-advised to acquaint themselves with causal methods from the potential outcomes framework.

Including covariates

The inclusion of a single additional time-varying covariate (TVC) \(Z\) is commonly done similarly to how “regular” \(X\) and \(Y\) outcomes are treated in the RI-CLPM; hence, rather than a bivariate RI-CLPM you would specify a tri- (or more-variate) RI-CLPM. As such, you decompose the TVC in within-components (e.g. \(wz1\), \(wz2\), etc.) and a between-component (a random intercept for \(Z\); e.g. \(RI_{z}\)) and model these components separately. However, if you want to control for many TVCs, this approach can quickly become unwieldly, leading to large statistical models that rely on many causal and statistical assumptions, and that can be hard to estimate. For researchers who want to adjust for a larger set of time-varying covariates, it might be worthwhile to consider alternative modeling approaches that have been proposed in the causal inference literature, such as those based on propensity scores.

Yes, it is statistically possible to run a “trivariate” RI-CLPM and empirical researchers have done so, see for example Van Lissa et al. (2019), Flouri et al. (2019), and Hygen et al. (2022). We don’t provide model code here for this mode, but extension to a trivariate RI-CLPM should be relatively straight forwards. That is, all variables should be decomposed into between- (random intercepts) and within-components, and relevant lagged effects, as well as (residual) variances and covariances, should be included in the model.

Moderation and mediation

There are various approaches for studying moderation within the RI-CLPM, depending on the measurement level of the moderator variable. Moderation by a time-invariant, categorical variable can be investigated using a multiple group RI-CLPM, as described in Extension 2 of Mulder and Hamaker (2021). Moderation by a time-varying, continuous variable can be investigating by creating latent interactions of the moderator \(\times\) within-components, or the moderator \(\times\) random intercept factor(s). See Ozkok et al. (2022) and Speyer et al. (2023) for detailed information on how to this.

Due to the high computational burden that the inclusion of a continuous time-varying moderator places on the estimation, Ozkok et al. (2022) make use of a Bayesian estimator. Their supplemental materials, including the Mplus model syntax, can be found at https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/suppl/10.1177/10944281211043733. They note that currently only Mplus supports the use of latent interactions as used here.

While it is statistically possible to include a third time-varying covariate into the RI-CLPM (which can be used to investigate mediation at the within-person level), there are multiple theoretical concerns that complicate the interpretation of estimates related to mediation. Researchers are strongly advised to give these concerns due consideration when interested in studying mediation using the RI-CLPM.

First, there are multiple options for defining a direct effect, and it might not be straightforward which option is appropriate for a particular research question. That is, there are multiple paths not through the mediator variable \(M\) that represent an effect of \(X\) on a \(Y\) at a later occasion. For example, is the direct effect only the lag-2 effect \(X_{1} \rightarrow Y_{3}\), or is the combination of the effects \(X_{1} \rightarrow Y_{2} \rightarrow Y_{3}\), \(X_{1} \rightarrow X_{2} \rightarrow Y_{3}\) and \(X_{1} \rightarrow Y_{3}\)?

Second, mediation analysis is often causal in nature, as you want to know the mechanism by which a variable \(X\) influences another variable \(Y\). For a causal interpretation of the statistical estimates, you need to carefully evaluate the causal identification assumptions (i.e., exchangeability, consistency, and positivity), and rely on the statistical (parametric) assumptions that implied by the RI-CLPM (e.g., linearity, multivariate normality). Evaluation of these assumptions can be quite challenging, and you might find that your targeted causal mediation effect is not identified. Interested readers are referred to Keele (2015) for a short introduction to these assumptions in the context of mediation analysis.

Standardization

In the RI-CLPM, the standardized effects are reflective/representative of how much within-person variance in \(y_{t}\) is uniquely explained (i.e., not also explained by other predictors) by the predictor \(x_{t-1}\). Please note that this does not imply that one can make a one-on-one comparison with the percentage of explained variance. However, the standardized effects can be used to compare which effect is relatively stronger (Schuurman et al. 2016).

The standardized lagged parameters are not equal to the (squared) semipartial correlation. Only under special circumstances, a standardized regression coefficient is equal to the pairwise correlation between the outcome and predictor, and the square of the standardized regression coefficient would then be the explained variance (\(R^{2}\)). This only applies in the case of (a) a simple regression, where there is only one predictor in the model, or (b) a multiple regression where the predictors are independent of each other. In the RI-CLPM, however, the multiple predictors are not independent of each other, and therefore the standardized regression coefficients are not equal to the pairwise correlation, (semi)partial correlation, or the unique explained variance.

However, the standardized coefficient is closely related to the semipartial correlation, as demonstrated in footnote 3 of Schuurman et al. (2016). The standardized coefficient is expressed in terms of pairwise correlations in the first equation there, and the semipartial correlation expressed in terms of pairwise correlations in the second equation. They are very similar, but there is a subtle difference in the denominator.

So, the semipartial correlation and standardized coefficient are not equal, but we can see that if the semipartical correlation for one predictor is larger than for another predictor, this will also be the case for the standardized coefficients of these predictors. The square of the semi-partial correlation is the unique explained variance, so we also know that the predictor with the largest standardized coefficient, will also have the largest unique explained variance.

Answer by dr. Noémi Schuurman.

While the lagged parameters can be constrained to be invariant over time, this does not imply that the standardized parameters will be invariant over time as well. The reason for this is that a standardized parameter is a function of the unstandardized parameter, and of the standard deviations of the predictor and the outcome variable, \(\beta_{standardized} = \frac{SD(x)}{SD(y)}\beta\). Since the variances of the within-components themselves are not constrained to be invariant over time, the standard deviations of the predictor and the outcome variable for the lagged effects can vary over time. Therefore, the standardized lagged effects can vary even while the unstandardized lagged effects are constrained to be equal over time.

To make the standardized lagged parameters time invariant, the within-person components need to have a variance of 1 at every occasion. In that case, the lagged parameters are already the standardized lagged parameters, and it becomes possible to impose time-constraints on them. At the first wave, the variances of the within-components can be set directly as these variables are exogenous. However, for \(t > 1\), the variances of the within-components are functions of the lagged parameters, and the covariance between the lagged predictors. Specifically, using the path tracing rules (Wright 1934) and assuming the variance of lagged within-component is in fact 1, the variance of \(wx_{it}\) can be expressed as \[Var[wx_{it}] = \alpha^{2}_{t} + \gamma^{2}_{t} + 2 \alpha_{t} \gamma_{t} Cov[wx_{i,t-1}, wy_{i,t-1}] + Var[u_{it}]\] where \(\alpha_{t}\) is the autoregressive effect of \(wx\), \(\gamma{t}\) is the cross-lagged effect from \(wy_{i,t-1}\) to \(wx_{it}\), and \(u_{Xit}\) is the residual of \(wx\). So, the variance of \(wx_{it}\) itself can be set to 1 by imposing a constrain on the residual variance \(Var[u_{it}]\), that is \[Var[u_{it}] = 1 - (\alpha^{2}_{t} + \gamma^{2}_{t} + 2 \alpha_{t} \gamma_{t} Cov[wx_{i,t-1}, wy_{i,t-1}])\].

The final quantity that we need to compute is the covariance (here also the correlation) between the two lagged within-components, \(Cov[wx_{i,t-1}, wy_{i,t-1}]\). At wave 1, this is simply a model parameter (i.e., the covariance between the within-components can be estimated directly). For \(t > 1\), this covariance is a function of the lagged coefficients, the variances of the lagged within-components, and the covariance between the residuals at the preceding occasion. Again, using the path tracing rules and assuming the variances of lagged within-components are in fact 1, it can be expressed as \[Cov[wx_{it}, wy_{it}] = \alpha_{t} \gamma_{t} + \beta_{t} \delta_{t} + \alpha_{t} \delta_{t} Cov[wx_{i,t-1}, wy_{i,t-1}] + \beta_{t} \gamma_{t} Cov[wx_{i,t-1}, wy_{i,t-1}] + Cov[u_{Xit}, u_{Yit}]\] where \(\delta_{t}\) is the autoregressive effect of \(wy\), \(\beta_{t}\) is the cross-lagged effect from \(wx_{i,t-1}\) to \(wy_{it}\), and \(u_{Yit}\) is the residual of \(wy\).

To impose such constraints in Mplus or lavaan, we thus first need to compute the covariance (i.e., correlation) between the within-components at each occasion. Then, these can be used to constrain the residual variances of the within-components in such a way that the variances of the within-person components equal 1. Syntax has been added to this website to specify this model in Mplus and lavaan. Note that in this model, the factor loadings that link the observed variables to the within-components are not fixed to 1 as is usually done, but they are estimated freely.

Answer by prof. dr. Ellen L. Hamaker.

Non-continuous outcomes

With Mplus version 8.7 and later, it is possible to estimate the RI-CLPM for binary and ordinal variables as well (Asparouhov and Muthén 2021). Compared to the RI-CLPM for continuous variables, there are two important differences:

  • A Bayesian estimator or WLSMV estimator (with theta parametrization) is needed. This can be specified by specifying either ESTIMATOR = BAYES; or ESTIMATOR = WLSMV; in the ANALYSIS command, as well as PARAM = THETA; for the WLSMV estimator.
  • The variances and residual variances of the within-components are fixed to 1, as is standard when using the probit-model in Mplus for categorical data analysis. These can be freely estimated in principle, but generally require larger sample sizes, and are prone to empirical non-identification. Several strategies for (partly) freeing up these variances are discussed in Asparouhov and Muthén (2021).

The interested reader is referred to Asparouhov and Muthén (2021) for more details.

Power

powRICLPM is an R-package that performs a power analysis for the RI-CLPM in a simple and user-friendly way. It implements the strategy as proposed by Mulder (2023), and includes various extensions such as the inclusion of measurement error (leading to the stable trait autoregressive trait state model by Kenny and Zautra (2001), and multiple constraints over time. powRICLPM is available from the Comprehensive R Archive Network (CRAN). More information can be found at https://jeroendmulder.github.io/powRICLPM/.

CLPM v. RICLPM

The autoregressive effects in the CLPM and the RI-CLPM capture two distinct phenomena. In the traditional CLPM, the autoregressive effects capture both within- and between-unit effects, and it can be interpreted as rank-order stability. In the RI-CLPM, the stable, between-person variance is separated from the within-person variance such that the autoregressive effects only capture within-person carry-over. In other words, in the RI-CLPM the autoregressive effects are typically smaller because the stable, trait-like stability is not captured anymore by the autoregressive effects (as in the CLPM), but by the random intercepts.

No, this is to be expected because the RI-CLPM is a more complex model than the CLPM. Therefore, the point estimates are less certain, but this does not imply that the estimates are biased and the model does not give us any insight in the underlying mechanism under study. In contrast, the RI-CLPM is likely less biased than results from the CLPM because the RI-CLM accounts for stable, unobserved factors that create lasting between-person differences, which can create spurious empirical relationships between the constructs under investigation when not properly accounted for.

Growth and development

The RI-CLPM estimates grand means at each time point. If these are constrained to be time-invariant, this would imply that on average there is no change. However, if these constraints cannot be imposed on the model, this means that on average there is some kind of change (e.g., growth, decline, U-shape, sudden jump between two waves, etc.). In this case, the model implies that every person is modeled to have the exact same underlying trend over time, but at a constant deviation from the average trend (as captured by the random intercept factor). Hence, instead of talking about constant means over time, one would have to talk about constant deviations from an average trend.

In the latent growth curve model (LGCM) and its extension, the latent curve model with structured residuals (LCM-SR), it is assumed that individual have the same kind of trend (e.g., a linear or quadratic trend), but there can be individual differences in the parameters of this trend; as a result, the differences between individuals change over time (i.e., they may increase or decrease). When the variance of the slope components (i.e., the slope factor of both \(X\) and \(Y\) in the bivariate case) is fixed to zero, the LGCM and LCS-SR become special cases of the RI-CLPM; if the grand means in the RI-CLPM are fixed to be invariant over time, then this is a special case of the LCM-SR (Usami, Murayama, and Hamaker 2019).

Response to Orth et al. (2021)

Lately, we have received numerous questions about statements regarding the use and appropriateness of the CLPM and RI-CLPM in “Testing Prospective Effects in Longitudinal Research: Comparing Seven Competing Cross-Lagged Models” by Orth et al. (2021). In our opinion, numerous conclusions herein are incorrect. Below we elaborate on some of their most prominent conclusions. For another reaction to this article, see https://replicationindex.com/2020/08/22/cross-lagged.

On page 1026, Orth et al. (2021) state that the RI-CLPM does not allow for the testing of prospective between-person effects because “(…) the random intercept factors provide information only about correlational associations between the constructs (…), but not about time-lagged (i.e, longitudinal) and directional between-person effects.” It is true that the RI-CLPM separates stable, between-person differences from temporal, within-person fluctuations. Only the latter allow one to look at prospective effects over time, while the stable between-person differences have no particular time-order.

The reasoning that Orth et al. (2021) seem to follow is this: If one is interested in trait-changes, we should not separate the stable trait components from the temporary fluctuations, as the first does not allow for the study of prospective effects and are not of interest because they are entirely stable, while the latter only contain state-like fluctuations that are less interesting to researchers who want to study in trait-changes. Orth et al. (2021) seem to consider both components separately not to be of interest, but nevertheless argue that a blend of both components is of key interest. We disagree with this reasoning.

Researchers who want to use prospective effects to study certain psychological processes, should make sure that their measurements are obtained at the time scale at which they believe the process of interest operates. If the trait-changes of interest are assumed to take place from one year to the next, then measuring yearly over several years should capture the fluctuations of interest within persons over the time span covered by the study. If trait-changes are assumed to take place at a less fine time scale, such as from decade to decade, this implies researchers should obtain data measured once per decade, for several decades.

Even if the stable, between-person differences are not of interest, these nevertheless need to be separated from the within-person fluctuations. The stable mean differences between persons are often referred to as trait differences, but may also be conceptualized as resulting from stable confounders, such as stable genetic and environmental factors that affect our measurements.

In sum, the focus should be on how the constructs are measured, specifically at what time scale they are measured. By definition, the random intercepts in the RI-CLPM capture variance that is stable over the duration of the study, which is why—from a data analytical perspective—these are referred to as traits. What remains is variance within persons over time, which is why—again from a data analytical perspective—this is referred to as state-like. These trait-like between-person differences and state-like, within-person fluctuations need not coincide perfectly with the trait-state distinction made by substantive researchers. Nevertheless, it is essential to separate these two sources, regardless of what they are called.

The characterization of the lagged effects as short- or long-term depends on the time scale at which the measurements are made. When using annual data, lagged effects capture the fluctuation from year to year; when dealing with data that were obtained every decade, the within-person fluctuations will concern the changes from decade to decade. It is all relative to how the data were measured, and even when we are focusing on trait changes by measuring people once every 10 years, for say 40 years, there will still be stable between-person differences between individuals that need to be controlled for.

Hence, the need to decompose the observations into a stable between-person component and temporal within-person components is independent of the time scale at which measurements were obtained.

We agree with the authors in so far that there should be a match between the research question and the statistical model used. However, the critical distinction here is not whether one is interested in within-person or between-person prospective effects (as argued above, the RI-CLPM can capture trait-changes depending on the time scale at which measurements are gathered). Rather, we should distinguish between descriptive, predictive, and causal research questions (Hamaker, Mulder, and Van IJzendoorn 2020). The CLPM is suitable for predictive research questions: e.g., “Who is at risk for heightened \(X\)?” and “Who should get an intervention to reduce \(Y\)?”. However, for answering causal research questions, other models should be used, because the CLPM does not control for stable, between-person difference. This is problematic as failing to properly account for such confounding effects can lead to finding spurious lagged effects, a failure to find true relationships, or more generally, biased estimates (Hamaker, Kuiper, and Grasman 2015).

On page 1028, Orth et al. (2021) state that “For the CLPM, the structural coefficients were much more consistent compared to the RI-CLPM, both across and within samples, which is important with regard to replicability of research findings (Asendorf et al., 2013). Even if theoretical consideration have priority over replicability in model selection, replicability of estimates is—other things being equal—an important criterion of the quality of statistical models.”

When the goal is prediction, this may indeed be an important consideration. However, when the goal is to gain insight in the underlying mechanism, we should not prefer a model simply based on its ability to replicate results: Replicating biased results is only giving us false confidence in erroneous conclusions.

Instead, we should consider which model seems more reasonable (e.g., do we believe there may be unobserved, stable confounding that plays a role in our measurements?), and which model provides a better description of the data (e.g., comparing competing models through the use of the chi-square difference test, or information criteria).

The larger standard errors associated with the RI-CLPM imply that if we want to have the higher precision and/or increase replicability, we need to obtain data from larger samples and/or at more waves.

References

Asparouhov, Tihomir, and Bengt Muthén. 2021. “Residual Structural Equation Models.” http://statmodel.com/download/Asparouhov_Muthen_2021a.pdf.
Flouri, Eirini, Efstathios Papachristou, Emily Midouhas, George B. Ploubidis, Glyn Lewis, and Heather Joshi. 2019. “Developmental Cascades of Internalising Symptoms, Externalising Problems and Cognitive Ability from Early Childhood to Middle Adolescence.” European Psychiatry 57 (April): 61–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.12.005.
Hamaker, Ellen L., Rebecca M. Kuiper, and Raoul P. P. P. Grasman. 2015. “A Critique of the Cross-Lagged Panel Model.” Psychological Methods 20 (1): 102–16. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889.
Hamaker, Ellen L., Jeroen D. Mulder, and Marinus H. Van IJzendoorn. 2020. “Description, Prediction and Causation: Methodological Challenges of Studying Child and Adolescent Development.” Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 46 (December): 100867. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100867.
Hygen, Beate W., Jay Belsky, Frode Stenseng, Silje Steinsbekk, Lars Wichstrøm, and Vera Skalicka. 2022. “Longitudinal Relations Between Gaming, Physical Activity, and Athletic Self-Esteem.” Computers in Human Behavior 132 (July): 107252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2022.107252.
Keele, Luke. 2015. “Causal Mediation Analysis: Warning! Assumptions Ahead.” American Journal of Evaluation 36 (4): 500–513. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098214015594689.
Kenny, David A., and Alex Zautra. 2001. “Trait–State Models for Longitudinal Data.” In New Methods for the Analysis of Change., edited by Linda M. Collins and Aline G. Sayer, 243–63. Washington: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10409-008.
Mulder, Jeroen D. 2023. “Power Analysis for the Random Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Model Using the powRICLPM R-Package.” Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 30 (4): 645–58. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2022.2122467.
Mulder, Jeroen D., and Ellen L. Hamaker. 2021. “Three Extensions of the Random Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Model.” Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 28 (4): 638–48. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1784738.
Orth, Ulrich, D. Angus Clark, M. Brent Donnellan, and Richard W. Robins. 2021. “Testing Prospective Effects in Longitudinal Research: Comparing Seven Competing Cross-Lagged Models.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 120 (4): 1013–34. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000358.
Ozkok, Ozlem, Manuel J. Vaulont, Michael J. Zyphur, Zhen Zhang, Kristopher J. Preacher, Peter Koval, and Yixia Zheng. 2022. “Interaction Effects in Cross-Lagged Panel Models: SEM with Latent Interactions Applied to Work-Family Conflict, Job Satisfaction, and Gender.” Organizational Research Methods 25 (4): 673–715. https://doi.org/10.1177/10944281211043733.
Schuurman, Noémi K., Emilio Ferrer, Mieke De Boer-Sonnenschein, and Ellen L. Hamaker. 2016. “How to Compare Cross-Lagged Associations in a Multilevel Autoregressive Model.” Psychological Methods 21 (2): 206–21. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000062.
Speyer, Lydia Gabriela, Anastasia Ushakova, Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, Aja Louise Murray, and Rogier Kievit. 2023. “Testing for Within × Within and Between × Within Moderation Using Random Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Models.” Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 30 (2): 315–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2022.2096613.
Usami, Satoshi, Kou Murayama, and Ellen L. Hamaker. 2019. “A Unified Framework of Longitudinal Models to Examine Reciprocal Relations.” Psychological Methods 24 (5): 637–57. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000210.
Van Lissa, Caspar J., Renske Keizer, Pol A. C. Van Lier, Wim H. J. Meeus, and Susan Branje. 2019. “The Role of Fathers’ Versus Mothers’ Parenting in Emotion-Regulation Development from Mid–Late Adolescence: Disentangling Between-Family Differences from Within-Family Effects.” Developmental Psychology 55 (2): 377–89. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000612.
Wright, Sewall. 1934. “The Method of Path Coefficients.” The Annals of Mathematical Statistics 5 (3): 161–215. https://doi.org/10.1214/aoms/1177732676.